Margaret Mead began her fieldwork
in Samoa under the instruction of Franz Boas. The goal was to demonstrate that
culture is relative, rather than concrete. Because of this very explicit goal,
Mead’s research has come under fire of criticism, stating that she may have
skewed her results to make a point. Indeed, she was searching for one example
to contradict the ideas of Freud. The work required to disprove a theory is
significantly less strenuous than to prove it. Freudian theory states that
adolescence is an unstable time due to biology, an idea that is still popular
today. Many teachers, parents, and therapists of adolescents will state that
young adults are emotionally turbulent by nature, due to the hormonal and
physical changes they are facing. Mead seeks out a culture, any culture, where
this is not true, so that she can disprove Freud. Her ultimate goal is to see
that even supposedly universal rules are culturally specific, and to answer the
question of whether the difficulties faced by young adults “were… due to being
adolescent or to being adolescent in America?” (pg 129).
Mead’s goal is admirable, and most
modern anthropologists will agree that the transformation from childhood to
adulthood is primarily a cultural construction. However, where I take issue is
not with her objective or her thesis, but with her motivation for choosing
Samoa as her research site. Mead explains, in some detail, what she was looking
for in a research site, as well as why she ultimately chose Samoa. Mead says
that to study culture, because we cannot use laboratory techniques, the
anthropologist must seek out other cultures that are different from our own.
Her claim is that the anthropologist should seek out simple civilizations,
stating “A primitive people without a written language present a much less
elaborate problem and a trained student can master the fundamental structure of
a primitive society in a few months” (pg 130). To me, this claim hearkens back
to the theory of cultural evolutionism, and would do shame to the Boasian
principle that there are no hierarchies to culture.
In Mead’s defense, it is clear that
some civilizations have fewer moving parts than others. In this way, they may simpler.
I will also defend the use of the word primitive, but only because of the time
at which Mead was writing. Clearly, this word would be inappropriate today, but
at the time what was meant was “less complex”, “tribal”, “hunter-gatherer”, or
equivalent. Nonetheless, the use of the word is consistent with word choice
from cultural evolutionists, and is likely to inspire in the reader feelings of
superiority to this inferior culture. In this way, Mead is reversing the
progress done by Boas. In addition, Mead’s claim that any culture, even a
simpler one, can be mastered in a matter of months strikes me as foolish and condescending.
Clearly, there are intricacies and nuances in any group that take more time
than mere months to learn. She also uses this argument to justify her use of
only fifty young women in her case study, claiming that the culture is
completely homogeneous. Even within a small group, there are differences between
individuals and families. If there is an opportunity to study a larger
population of the group, it should be seized. Indeed, some of the most
interesting conclusions about a society come not from the majority, or “Joe
Normal,” but from the outliers and how they navigate their unique cultural
struggles.
Mead also seeks to use her
fieldwork to inform American culture. To teach the average American, not just
fellow anthropologists, that we can learn from vastly different societies how
to better our own, and to demonstrate that what was previously considered
biologically determined is in fact highly influenced by culture. In this way,
she is comparing cultures, and thereby deviating from the beliefs of Boas.
However, this deviation I support. Though it is important to attempt to see a
culture without our own cultural lens, without some amount of comparison
anthropology is simply knowledge for knowledge’s sake. Mead’s work delves into
a new use of anthropology to inform and improve our own cultural identity.
I greatly admire Mead’s work, and
her ability to see that American culture can be improved upon through
understanding of exotic cultures. However, she spent so much time travelling
between groups and attempting to master them so quickly, I have to wonder what
she might have learn had she stayed with one group throughout her career.
Clearly she was doing something right, as her contributions to anthropology are
undeniable in their importance, but I wonder how her view of each group may
have changed with more depth and less breadth.
I really liked your entry and think that a lot (or all) of the issues that you brought up are true with her work; how in depth could her work be if she wasn’t spending too much time in these places, choosing places because they would be easier and simpler to learn from, possibly veering from Boas. You also brought up something Mead said that I had not realized before, that being “because we cannot use laboratory techniques, the anthropologist must seek out other cultures that are different from our own.” This quote got me thinking; I understand how difficult it would have been, or is, to use empirical science or laboratory techniques to study a culture, but I wouldn’t say that we should entirely dismiss the scientific process.
ReplyDeleteI feel like while it may be difficult to completely perform an experimental study of a culture or various cultures, it may still be possible to use some ideas which are significant when conducting scientific experiments to better understand and analyze culture. By actively choosing a society which she thinks will be the opposite of America seems to undermine her cause, as she almost chooses a society based on expected results. It seems as if she chose Samoa because it would be a simple culture to learn from and also would give her expected results. You must only find one counter example to discredit a theory, but it may have been more beneficial to randomly choose a number of cultures to compare. This would have made her study more reliable and credible. However, I know that it would close to impossible to study multiple cultures exploring the idea of adolescence angst, because then you would run into the problem of the depth of the study in each culture, which you mentioned above. But by only studying a few individuals within one culture (and a culture she has actively chosen for expected results) then how much can we trust her outcome? I also wonder if it would have made more of a statement if Mead had found a difference of adolescence psychological development in a culture which wasn’t as drastically different as Samoa. I imagine that there are various cultures that Americans are more familiar with that, in some way, have a different way of handling adolescent girls. This may have spared her work from some scrutiny.
I understand that Mead was critiqued on the grounds that she was more of a public figure than an academic and because of the objective of her study, but I agree with you that it is more than her position that is a flaw in her work. Her reasoning behind choosing the location seems biased and possibly evolutionist. I completely agree with your critiques of Mead’s work, and your blog also helped me to think about and understand the common critiques of her work as well.